Reading the World, Reading Each Other
As you move through life, you âread the world.â More than anything, this means you read the people you encounter. You interpret their faces, gestures and moods based on subtle behavioral cues. You can fathom âthe deepest aspects of the minds of othersâ because of brain cells called mirror neurons, which are âhighly and narrowly specialized.â
âMirror neurons undoubtedly provide, for the first time in history, a plausible neurophysiological explanation for complex forms of social cognition and interaction.â
When mirror neurons discharge, they set off a kind of imitative impulse in your brain that reflects what you perceive others to be doing. This is so finely tuned that the mirror neurons fire differently depending on the intention of an action. For example, if you see someone reach for a cup in the presence of contextual cues indicating that he or she is about to drink from it, your mirror neurons will fire differently from the way they would if the cues indicated that he or she were picking it up to clear the table. Cues may be visual â you might see steam rising from the cup, for example â or auditory â you might hear a dishwasher running in the kitchen.
âModern science requires...technological innovations, management skills and lots of money.â
Mirroring and interpretation are crucial parts of learning. Humans imitate one another almost as soon as they are born: One study showed a newborn imitating adultsâ expressions when he was only â41 minutes old.â Children play together by imitating one anotherâs behavior, even before they can speak. When theyâre older, they imitate on a more complex level; this is how people pass on cultural practices. Unfortunately for parents, children are so acutely attuned to imitation that theyâll copy what you do, rather than do what you say.
âI think one of the primary goals of imitation may actually be the facilitation of an embodied âintimacyâ between the self and others during social relations.â
Experience also shapes your neurological response to the world. One study showed that groups with different physical experiences responded differently to images: Dancersâ brains responded more intensely to videos of dancers, while practitioners of capoeira, a Brazilian martial art, responded more intensely to videos of their practice. Researchers who study advertising have noted a neurological sense of âaffiliation,â whereby people perceive themselves as being similar to the people they see in ads and respond powerfully to them.
The Discovery of Mirror Neurons
Like many scientific breakthroughs, mirror neurons were discovered by accident. Giacomo Rizzolatti was leading a team of neurophysiologists in Parma, Italy, who were studying brain activity in macaque monkeys. They were monitoring cells in the brainâs âpremotor cortex,â which is dedicated to âplanning, selecting and executing actions.â When they inserted electrodes into the monkeysâ brains to monitor âmotor cellâ activity during tasks, they received an unexpected response. An experimenter did something within sight of a monitored monkey, and the monkeyâs nerve cell fired as though the monkey itself had performed the action.
âThe mind is not a book. I do not think we âreadâ other minds, and we should stop using terms that already contain bias about the way we think about such a process.â
The existing theory could not account for this. It said that different parts of the brain related to different functions: Some parts controlled perception, some action and so on. Yet the monkeyâs neurons seemed to be responding more holistically, as if action and perception were closely tied â and as if the monkeyâs perceptions of othersâ actions were linked to its own actions. Additional research showed that this was indeed the case: The neurons fired in response to particular actions. For example, they fired in response to a hand grasping food but not to a hand miming the act of grasping food, indicating that the monkeys perceived the actionâs purpose on a neurological level. And the neurons fired differently when the hand grasped larger objects, such as apples, from when it grasped smaller objects, such as raisins.
âEmbodied Semanticsâ
Researchers realized that meaning does not reside in abstractions but rather in the details of and interactions among internal and external phenomena. Like monkeys, humans are aware of intention on a neurological level, as if they can read one anotherâs minds. Older theories of human and animal intelligence didnât value imitation. More recent theories, though, reverse this, asserting that the ability to imitate is both central to intelligence and a good measure of it. Itâs also an important part of communication and language.
âDuring conversation, we imitate each otherâs expressions, even each otherâs syntactic constructions.â
Watch people talk. They use more than words. They wince, smile, pause and gesture. âIconic gesturesâ parallel the content of the words the speaker uses, while âbeat gesturesâ establish the timing and rhythm of speech. Beat gestures help the speaker keep track of what he or she is saying, while iconic gestures help the listeners, whose mirror neurons fire as though they themselves were performing those actions. When you use figurative language that refers to the body, such as the phrase âgrasping a concept,â the motor neurons that you would use in grasping fire. Physical movements facilitate linguistic communication because of this motor neuron activity in response to language.
âMimicking others is not just a form of communicating nonverbally; it helps us perceive othersâ expressions (and therefore their emotions) in the first place.â
A powerful developmental link exists âbetween hand and mouth early in life.â Gestures precede language. Studies of which neurons fire when people observe gestures indicate that mirror neurons are central to âlanguage development and language evolution.â When a childâs gestures and speech donât match, thatâs a sign that theyâre wrestling with ânew concepts.â
âAlmost instinctively we humans tend to synchronize our movements.â
All of this leads to the linguistic theory of âembodied semantics,â which sees âlinguistic conceptsâ as developing from specific âsensory-motorâ realities. Mirror neuronsâ role in language helps explain why people find taking part in a conversation easier than listening to a monologue. If speech flowed outward from the isolated self, listening to a monologue would be easier. However, people find conversations easier, because they synchronize with one another neurologically, echoing one another through mirror neurons.
Mirror Neurons and Emotions
If you ever watched an emotionally charged sporting event that had a lot riding on the outcome, such as the World Cup final or game seven of the World Series, you probably recall important moments from that contest vividly. When you do, you may feel the same surge of elation or disappointment that you felt when you were actually watching the game. Your emotions arenât just those of a spectator; you probably feel empathy with the players themselves, wincing with their pain even in memory. This happens because of the âneural mechanism for mirroring in the brain.â Empathy plays a central role in human society and interaction.
âAt least six different laboratories using a variety of techniques for studying the human brain have recently confirmed deficits in mirror neuron areas in individuals with autism.â
When people interact, they indicate through more than their words that they understand one another. They tend to synchronize their expressions and gestures. Research has shown that in couples, the more intense the âmotor synchronyâ the greater the âemotional rapport.â Mirroring others helps you to understand their emotions. When people look at pictures of faces expressing various emotions, their own faces echo the expressions in the pictures. If individuals are unable to move their faces freely, for example, if they are holding a pen between their teeth, limiting movement, they are markedly less able to identify what another personâs expression communicates.
Mirror Neurons and Medicine
One of the researchersâ original purposes for studying motor neurons in macaque monkeys was to find ways to help humans whoâd lost some âmotor functions after brain damage.â In mirror-neuron research, scientists have found hope for treating other conditions as well. Understanding the role of mirror neurons in cravings may improve treatment of addiction, and mirror-neuron research holds great promise for the treatment of autism.
âGiven the high rate of relapse for practically every known form of addiction, a better understanding of the role of mirror neurons in relapses will be extremely important in the treatment of addictive behaviors.â
Autistic children have visual focuses different from those of children who are developing normally. One early sign of an autism-related condition is that the young child shows âsevere deficits in social relations.â One hypothesis says that this is because their mirror neurons do not function as they should. Thus imitation, one of the functions of mirror-neuron activity, may work as part of a therapy for autism. The therapist would imitate the autistic childâs actions to stimulate his or her mirror neurons. Eventually, this would lead the child out of isolation and into synchronization, and social and emotional harmony with others.
Politics, Philosophy and the End of Individualism
In the late 1990s, a political science graduate student, Darren Schreiber, undertook research at the Brain Mapping Center at the University of California San Diego to test âtheories about how political attitudes are formedâ and to determine whether people make political decisions rationally. An established body of observations about survey data showed differences in how quickly people responded to political questions. Respondents with clear political positions tended to answer questions quickly, and their answers were usually consistent: A liberal answer on one question predicted a liberal answer on others. Respondents without political affiliations took longer to answer, and their answers tended to scatter across the political spectrum. Theorists suggested that the difference in speed corresponded to different processing procedures in the brain for familiar tasks than for new ones.
âA healthy democracy, in my opinion, needs mechanisms of empathy and identification between the people and their political representatives.â
Schreiber used brain imaging to monitor neural activity in two groups of college students, âpolitical junkiesâ and ânovices,â as they looked at pictures of faces. The political junkiesâ brains showed activity that indicated they felt a âsense of belongingâ when they viewed pictures of politicians, while the brains of the novices, who did not recognize the politicians, showed no change. In another study, researchers presented the subjects with a series of politically charged statements. The paradoxical results surprised the researchers. In the brains of practiced political thinkers, elements of the âdefault state networkâ kicked in, which happens when people are completing mundane tasks. Essentially, their expertise enabled them to respond to the statements without much new thought. The more they knew, the less reasoning took place. In contrast, the novices âgeared up for cognition.â
âMirror neurons are the brain cells that fill the gap between self and other by enabling some sort of simulation or inner imitation of the actions of others.â
Another study examined the sort of brain activity that occurs during different kinds of social relationships. Subjects watched videos in which people demonstrated different modes of interaction: one showed âcommunal sharing,â with kind and egalitarian interactions, while another demonstrated âhierarchical inequalities.â When subjects watched these videos, brain scans showed that both their mirror neurons and their default state networks were highly engaged, indicating both that they were empathizing with the people in the videos and that paying attention to social relationships was routine â a default state.
âWe are wired for empathy, which should inspire us to shape our society and make it a better place to live.â
These results have implications for politics and philosophy. Western philosophy assumes an autonomous, rational self. The 17th-century French philosopher Rene Descartes summed up this idea with the Latin phrase, âCogito ergo sumâ (âI think, therefore I amâ). The problem with this perspective is that it ignores intersubjectivity â the meaning produced through interaction. Various thinkers have attempted to explain how humans understand one another. Some believe they do it using analogies: individuals understand themselves, then extrapolate to others. However, this idea has two problems: Most people arenât that self-aware; and analogical thinking requires a great deal of conscious reasoning, while most people understand one another quickly and intuitively.
The âneural mechanisms of mirroring and simulationâ are what enable people to connect. They also explain intersubjectivity and mutual understanding. Mirror-neuron studies show that humans arenât isolated individuals, as Western thought has traditionally portrayed them, but rather that they are connected on a neurological level. The wiring in the human brain predisposes people to interact and share experiences. âExistential neuroscienceâ shows that humans generate meaning not through individual effort but rather through mutuality.
Mirror-neuron studies may also illuminate the effects of violence in the media. Behavioral studies of children who see media violence have come to a variety of conclusions, including the disturbing one that violence may increase among children who watch violent media â although childrenâs environments can mitigate some of this effect. New understandings of mirror neurons underscore the likelihood that people will imitate actions that they observe. After all, the function of mirror neurons is to promote imitation on a cellular level. Peopleâs influence on one another, right down to the level of cellular activity, is a direct challenge to long-established models of personal responsibility and autonomy.